Skip to main content
Home
plus.maths.org

Secondary menu

  • My list
  • About Plus
  • Sponsors
  • Subscribe
  • Contact Us
  • Log in
  • Main navigation

  • Home
  • Articles
  • Collections
  • Podcasts
  • Maths in a minute
  • Puzzles
  • Videos
  • Topics and tags
  • For

    • cat icon
      Curiosity
    • newspaper icon
      Media
    • graduation icon
      Education
    • briefcase icon
      Policy

      Popular topics and tags

      Shapes

      • Geometry
      • Vectors and matrices
      • Topology
      • Networks and graph theory
      • Fractals

      Numbers

      • Number theory
      • Arithmetic
      • Prime numbers
      • Fermat's last theorem
      • Cryptography

      Computing and information

      • Quantum computing
      • Complexity
      • Information theory
      • Artificial intelligence and machine learning
      • Algorithm

      Data and probability

      • Statistics
      • Probability and uncertainty
      • Randomness

      Abstract structures

      • Symmetry
      • Algebra and group theory
      • Vectors and matrices

      Physics

      • Fluid dynamics
      • Quantum physics
      • General relativity, gravity and black holes
      • Entropy and thermodynamics
      • String theory and quantum gravity

      Arts, humanities and sport

      • History and philosophy of mathematics
      • Art and Music
      • Language
      • Sport

      Logic, proof and strategy

      • Logic
      • Proof
      • Game theory

      Calculus and analysis

      • Differential equations
      • Calculus

      Towards applications

      • Mathematical modelling
      • Dynamical systems and Chaos

      Applications

      • Medicine and health
      • Epidemiology
      • Biology
      • Economics and finance
      • Engineering and architecture
      • Weather forecasting
      • Climate change

      Understanding of mathematics

      • Public understanding of mathematics
      • Education

      Get your maths quickly

      • Maths in a minute

      Main menu

    • Home
    • Articles
    • Collections
    • Podcasts
    • Maths in a minute
    • Puzzles
    • Videos
    • Topics and tags
    • Audiences

      • cat icon
        Curiosity
      • newspaper icon
        Media
      • graduation icon
        Education
      • briefcase icon
        Policy

      Secondary menu

    • My list
    • About Plus
    • Sponsors
    • Subscribe
    • Contact Us
    • Log in
    • Outer space: Thinking inside the box

      John D. Barrow
      12 July, 2010
      7 comments


      After my book 100 Essential Things You Didn't Know You Didn't Know was published I received lots of emails and letters asking about some of the mathematical applications and issues that appeared there. One topic was overwhelmingly the most common. Some people didn't understand it; others didn't believe it. Some puzzled correspondents were just interested readers, but at least one was a very famous professor of physics. What worried them so much was the infamous Three-box, or Monty Hall, problem in chapter 30.

      Did you pick the right box or should you switch?

      You may remember how it goes. Imagine you're a contestant in a game show, faced with three boxes, only one of which contains a prize. You have to pick the prize-winning box. The game show host, who can see all the boxes' contents, then opens one of the empty boxes and asks if you want to keep to your original choice or switch boxes. What should you do? The answer is that you should always switch. There was only a 1/3 chance that your original choice was right and a 2/3 chance that it was wrong and the prize was in the other two boxes. One was opened and shown to be empty, so there is a 2/3 probability the prize is in the other box you didn't choose — switch to it! (You can find out more in the Plus article Thomas Bayes & Mr Zootpooper.)

      This problem is not paradoxical — just counter intuitive — but it's a good warm-up for another problem that is much more perplexing. This time there are just two boxes. In one of them there is a prize of some value and in the other another prize of twice the value. Pick one of the boxes and open it. You are now offered the chance to switch your choice to the other box. What should you do?

      The problem is that you don't know if you opened the box containing the smaller or the larger prize. So let's see what the expected pay-off is if you switch after opening one box to find it contains a prize of value V. If it was the smaller prize, then by switching you would get 2V. If it was the larger prize, you would end up with V/2. So, on average, a switch will bring you an expected prize value of 1/2 × 2V + 1/2 × V/2 = 5V/4. This is bigger than V so you always gain by switching.

      This is very odd because if you always gain by switching you didn't need to look in the box, did you? Always switch. And after you've switched, the same argument applies and you should switch again! I learnt about this horrible paradox in an article by past Plus contributor Steven Brams and his colleague Marc Kilgour. It makes the three-box problem seem totally obvious. What do you make of it? An interesting clue is to start by asking what would happen if the prizes were actually £5 and £10 (or 5and10) banknotes?

      • Log in or register to post comments

      Comments

      Anonymous

      17 August 2010

      Permalink

      Why do you say that you "always gain" by switching if the 5/4V was computed as an average? Wouldn't it be proper to say that "on average you always gain" but that you simply don't know if you gain or not for any given switch?

      • Log in or register to post comments

      Anonymous

      27 August 2010

      Permalink

      You arrived at the average result of 5v/4 by saying that the boxes contain 2v and v/2; but they don't: they contain 2v and v OR v and v/2.

      • Log in or register to post comments

      Anonymous

      29 September 2010

      In reply to 2 Box Switch Problem by Anonymous

      Permalink

      Say that you are told that one of the boxes contain aandtheothercontainstwiceofthat2a. Consider the example where you open a box and find 100init.Nowyouaren′tsurewhetherthisisequalto2aora(i.eifyoupickedthea box then the other contains 200,orifyoupickedthe2a box then the other contains only $50.).

      The expected value is 5/4aifyoudecidetoswitch(125).

      • Log in or register to post comments

      Anonymous

      29 October 2010

      In reply to 2 Box Switch Problem by Anonymous

      Permalink

      On further thought, you're right: they contain 2v and v OR v and v/2. The mistake in the author's reasoning is to consider the expectation based on a variable starting point. This will lead to a factor of 4 in the two sums of money (2x and 1/2x).

      • Log in or register to post comments

      Anonymous

      30 August 2010

      Permalink

      To follow up on 1st anon:

      The boxes contain 2V and V reward. Half the time you open the 2V and half the time the V box.

      If you never switch, on average, you get 3V/2 reward

      If you always switch, on average, you get ... 3V/2 reward.

      You can't have a strategy in this game, unless you have some idea of what range of values constitutes a large and small reward.

      The monty hall problem works because the game master has extra information which he is revealing by opening a door. No such extra information is being revealed here.

      -Kaushik Ghose

      • Log in or register to post comments

      Anonymous

      20 September 2010

      Permalink

      It seems that it doesn't matter, whether you switch or not - you get the same prize on the average. An excel calculation using random numbers in the range [0,1) with 2 columns and 20 rows, where the smaller number in row was considered to be of value 1 and the bigger number was considered to be of value 2 showed it explicitly that you get the same amount whatever is your strategy (it was excatly 1,5 average for 20 pairs of numbers). The problem is thus equivalent to coin tossing.

      Gleb Shiroki

      • Log in or register to post comments

      Anonymous

      22 November 2010

      In reply to Thinking inside the box by Anonymous

      Permalink

      In the example, V is being used to represent both the larger prize and the smaller prize.This can't be correct. If V is fixed at the value of the smaller prize the average return for switching is 3V/2. This is the same as the return for not switching.

      • Log in or register to post comments

      Read more about...

      bayes theorem
      three door problem
      outerspace
      University of Cambridge logo

      Plus Magazine is part of the family of activities in the Millennium Mathematics Project.
      Copyright © 1997 - 2025. University of Cambridge. All rights reserved.

      Terms